The growing animosity between Democrats and Republicans in America is a well-discussed problem. It is frequently covered in the media, academic articles, and blog posts (hey, like this one!). A term academic research uses to describe the problem is “affective polarization.” Affective polarization captures the extent to which citizens feel sympathy towards partisan in- groups and antagonism towards partisan out-groups (Wagner 2021). Basically, it means that the “other side” isn’t viewed just an electoral rival to argue policies with, but an enemy that poses an existential threat.
What’s fascinating (or perhaps a bit alarming) is that the rise in affective partisanship is less about supporting or disagreeing on matters of legislative policy like the tax rate, and instead is about our partisanship becoming key component of our identity increasingly on par or even surpassing characteristics such as gender, race, religion, and nationality (Iyengar and Westwood 2015). Some research has begun referring to partisanship as a “super identity” due to the high overlap between specific demographic groups (like the four mentioned above) and partisanship (Mason 2018).
Importantly, this growing issue of a partisan super identity can be found not “only” in individual voters, but also in our elected officials (Abramowitz 2010; Utych 2020; Desilver 2022). But here’s the catch: research has found that average Americans are less polarized when it comes to policy positions than we think and act like we are, though actual polarization is getting worse as well (Levendusky and Malhotra 2016; Westfall et. al 2015).
As some political scientist, myself included, like to jokingly put it, affective partisanship is a heck of a drug. Identifying with a partisan group exerts a powerful influence on our behaviors and views.
If you were wondering how affective partisanship influences our life and reality, it affects[1]:
- The media we consume (Levendusky 2013; Druckman al 2018)
- Our consumer behavior (Gerber and Huber 2009; McConnell et 2018)
- What we think about each other (Garrett al 2014; Levendusky and Malhotra 2016; Levendusky 2018; Ahler & Sood 2018; Wojcieszak and Warner 2020)
- Who we find attractive (Nicholson et 2016)
- Who we make friends with or if we can make friends where we live (Gimpel & Hui 2015; Bakshy et al. 2015; Chopik & Motyl 2016; Pew 2017)
- Who we date or marry (Huber and Malhotra 2017; Iyengar al 2012; Iyengar et al. 2019)
- Where we want to live (Bishop 2009; Gimpel and Hui 2015; Truglia and Cruces 2017)
- How we spend time with family we disagree with politically (Oliphant and Smith 2016; Tavernise and Seelye 2016; Chen and Rohla 2018)
- Who we hire (Gift and Gift 2015)
- Who we trust (Carlin and Love 2018; Iyengar and Westood 2015)
- Who we deem worthy of scholarships (Iyengar and Westwood 2015)
- Attitudes towards science and public health (Allcott al 2020; YouGov 2022)
- How we interpret or understand the world (Lakoff 2002)
- …and in an Illinois-specific example, whether the Chicago area and downstate Illinois should even be in the same state (Jackson and Foster 2021).
What many researchers find especially disheartening about all this is that our views of the demographic makeup and policy positions of the “other” party are wildly inaccurate (Levendusky and Malhotra 2016; Ahler & Sood 2018; Settle 2018). One explanation for these misperceptions might be that research has found the most visible people in American politics are the most ideologically extreme (Levendusky & Malhotra 2016; Settle 2018; Blazina 2022). So we’re seeing the extreme minority of parties and interpret them as being representative when they’re not, at least not yet.
Worse, the consequences of affective partisanship extend far beyond our social spheres. Research has found that in recent years we have become more “morally disengaged,” essentially dehumanizing and vilifying people in the “other” party (Cassesse 2020; Martherhus et. al 2021; Pacilli et al. 2016), including some voters expressing enjoyment when the “other side” suffers (Combs et. al 2009; Kalmoe and Mason 2019).
Occasionally, this can even extend into open hostility: researchers have found that a growing segment of the American electorate is comfortable resorting to committing violence against political opponents (Kalmoe and Mason 2022), and experimental work shows that stronger partisans are angrier when their group loses an election, with that anger spurring political participation and manifesting itself into increased levels of hostility towards opponents (Huddy et al. 2015; Valentino et al. 2011; Mason 2018).
Lastly, and perhaps most importantly, research has found that affective partisanship damages dedication to democratic values and democracy in Americans (Graham and Svolik 2020). The actions at June 14th 2017 practice for the annual congressional charity baseball game or what happened at the U.S. capitol building on January 6th 2021 certainly come to mind thinking about these findings and the implications for affective polarization on American society.
So what can we do about affective partisanship? That’s a big question that doesn’t necessarily have clear answers. But we do have some research that may be helpful: First, exercising wariness around partisan news sources is a good first step, as this type of media may impact affective partisanship (Lelkes et al. 2017; Druckman et. al 2018; Broockman and Kalla 2022). This may be because partisan news sources often depict the opposing party in extreme terms that dehumanize or link them to extreme groups like Nazis and Communists (Berry & Sobieraj 2014) and focuses disproportionately on out-party scandals (real or imagined), encouraging hostility toward the “other” party (Puglisi & Snyder 2011). Additionally, the lack of balanced content in these outlets may drive viewers to adopt extreme ideological positions (Levendusky 2013), which, in turn, may increase affective polarization (Rogowski and Sutherland 2016, Webster and Abramowitz 2017). This all may be contributing to affective partisanship and particularly the “moral disengagement” component discussed earlier, which forms the bedrock of further negative behaviors.
Social media is another vehicle for affective partisanship and polarization, as well. Considering that more extreme partisans post on social media accounts, users of social media may be more exposed to affective partisanship and maybe have skewed views of the “other” party (Bakshy et. al 2015; Levendusky & Malhotra 2016; Settle 2018; Blazina 2022). Correcting misperceptions about the demographic makeup of the “other” party decreases affective polarization (Ahler and Sood 2018), although fighting with strangers on the internet is hardly a productive use of one’s time. Rather, being cognizant of the fact that algorithms boost extreme content, and being aware of how your own posts contribute to perceptions of both “your” side and the “other” side may help with affective polarization.
One way to address this, besides seeking out detailed demographic data about the “other” party (which, who wouldn’t want to do that?!), is to break out of the so-called “echochambers” we’ve put ourselves in and seek out social interactions with members of the “other” party, which have been found to lessen affective polarization (Mutz 2002; Nelson 2015; Warner and Villamil 2017; Wojcieszak and Warner 2020; Baron et. al 2021; Levendusky and Stecula 2021; Rossiter 2021).
For example, if someone said a certain political party believed something extreme, in the past you’d be able to say “I know Dave from bowling is from that party and he doesn’t think that extreme thing.” Absent those non-political social connections that have decreased in recent years we fill our knowledge of outparties with the extreme examples we see in media or social media. Get to know “Dave” again. And, importantly, the type of contact matters. Cooperation helps with the impact of such interactions with the other party (Allport, 1954; Wojcieszak and Warner 2020). So perhaps find ways to cooperate with members of the “other” party outside of political areas— again, such as playing on a bowling team with people whose political views are different than one’s own. As well, being trained on personal persuasion techniques makes us more open to listening to the other side and has been found to decrease affective partisanship (Kalla and Broockman 2021). So maybe we all just need to work on our communication skills?
In total, affective partisanship is a growing problem in America. Its impacts are wide reaching and potentially deadly, though actual policy distances are actually smaller than people perceive them, but they are growing. Our understanding of solutions are in the nascent stages, but there appears to be some things that MAY help solve this problem like media consumption, social media engagement, social networks, and communications training.
Additional interesting data, for those of you who love numbers:
- In 2015, less than a quarter of dating profiles had information about party identification but in 2017 that rate had doubled (Kiefer 2017).
- In 1958, 33 percent of Democrats wanted their children to marry a Democrat, and 25 percent of Republicans wanted their children to marry a But by 2016, 60 percent of Democrats and 63 percent of Republicans felt that way (Vavreck 2017).
- Thanksgiving dinners attended by people from areas who voted for different political parties are 30 to 50 minutes shorter than dinners attended by people from areas that voted for the same political party (Chen and Rohla 2018).
- Further, Democrats and Republicans have “just a few” or no friends in the opposing party (Pew Research Center 2017).
- As recently as prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, about 80% of Democrats and two-thirds of Republicans have said yes parents should have to vaccinate their children against infectious It’s now 77% of Democrats and 39% of Republicans (YouGov 2022).
- When it comes to discussions around Downstate Illinois splitting off from the Chicago area, 48% of Republicans report supporting the idea compared to 15% of Democrats (Jackson and Foster 2021). Though of course, supporting such an idea in abstract and doing so if the possibility were to become more real are two very different things.
- Receiving training on personal persuasion has been found to decrease affective partisanship through humanizing others, the formation of emotional connections during conversations, and seeing members of the “other” party as different than their stereotypes (Kalla and Broockman 2021).
- This is all important as average Americans are actually less polarized than affective polarization and misperceptions would have us believe, though the actual distance on policy issues is growing (Levendusky and Malhotra 2016; Westfall et. al 2015).
Have thoughts about effective tactics for reducing affective partisanship in your daily life? Let us know in the comments. As well, stay tuned to the Capitol Connection and the CSPL email list for updates on the work we’re doing in this area, which includes research, webinars, and policy summits.
________________________________________
Author biography: AJ Simmons is the Research Director of the Center for State Policy and Leadership at UIS. He holds a PhD from the School of Politics and Global Studies at Arizona State University. He likes bowling and discussing politics with people he disagrees with.
Bibliography
Abramowitz, A.I., 2010. Transformation and polarization: The 2008 presidential election and the new American electorate. Electoral Studies, 29(4), pp.594-603.
Ahler, D.J. and Sood, G., 2018. The parties in our heads: Misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. The Journal of Politics, 80(3), pp.964-981.
Allcott, H., Boxell, L., Conway, J., Gentzkow, M., Thaler, M. and Yang, D., 2020. Polarization and public health: Partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus
pandemic. Journal of public economics, 191, p.104254. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Perseus Books.
Bakshy, E., Messing, S. and Adamic, L.A., 2015. Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook. Science, 348(6239), pp.1130-1132.
Baron, H., Blair, R., Choi, D.D., Gamboa, L., Gottlieb, J., Robinson, A.L., Rosenzweig, S., Turnbull, M. and West, E.A., 2021. Can Americans Depolarize? Assessing the Effects of Reciprocal Group Reflection on Partisan Polarization.
Berry, J.M. and Sobieraj, S., 2013. The outrage industry: Political opinion media and the new incivility. Oxford University Press.
Bishop, B., 2009. The big sort: Why the clustering of like-minded America is tearing us apart. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Blazina, Carrie. 2022. Americans at the ends of the ideological spectrum are the most active in national politics. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact- tank/2022/01/05/americans-at-the-ends-of-the-ideological-spectrum-are-the-most-active-in- national-politics/
Broockman, D. and Kall, J. 2022. The manifold effects of partisan media on viewers’ beliefs and attitudes: A field experiment with Fox News viewers. OSF Preprints. https://osf.io/jrw26/
Carlin, R.E. and Love, G.J., 2018. Political competition, partisanship and interpersonal trust in electoral democracies. British Journal of Political Science, 48(1), pp.115-139.
Cassese, E.C., 2020. Dehumanization of the opposition in political campaigns. Social Science Quarterly, 101(1), pp.107-120.
Chopik, W.J. and Motyl, M., 2016. Ideological fit enhances interpersonal orientations. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(8), pp.759-768.
Combs, D. J., Powell, C. A., Schurtz, D. R., & Smith, R. H. 2009. “Politics, schadenfreude, and ingroup identification: The sometimes happy thing about a poor economy and death.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 635-646.
DeSilver, Drew. 2022. The Polarizaition in Today’s Congress Has Roots Going Back Decades. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2022/03/10/the-polarization-in- todays-congress-has-roots-that-go-back-decades/
Druckman, J.N., Levendusky, M.S. and McLain, A., 2018. No need to watch: How the effects of partisan media can spread via interpersonal discussions. American Journal of Political
Science, 62(1), pp.99-112.
Garrett, R.K., Gvirsman, S.D., Johnson, B.K., Tsfati, Y., Neo, R. and Dal, A., 2014. Implications of pro-and counterattitudinal information exposure for affective polarization. Human communication research, 40(3), pp.309-332.
Gerber, A.S. and Huber, G.A., 2009. Partisanship and economic behavior: Do partisan differences in economic forecasts predict real economic behavior?. American Political Science Review, 103(3), pp.407-426.
Gift, K. and Gift, T., 2015. Does politics influence hiring? Evidence from a randomized experiment. Political Behavior, 37(3), pp.653-675.
Gidron, N., Adams, J. and Horne, W., 2019. Toward a comparative research agenda on affective polarization in mass publics. APSA Comparative Politics Newsletter, 29, pp.30-36.
Gimpel, J.G. and Hui, I.S., 2015. Seeking politically compatible neighbors? The role of neighborhood partisan composition in residential sorting. Political Geography, 48, pp.130-142.
Graham, M.H. and Svolik, M.W., 2020. Democracy in America? Partisanship, polarization, and the robustness of support for democracy in the United States. American Political Science Review, 114(2), pp.392-409.
Huddy, L., Mason, L. and Aarøe, L., 2015. Expressive partisanship: Campaign involvement, political emotion, and partisan identity. American Political Science Review, 109(1), pp.1-17.
Huber, G.A. and Malhotra, N., 2017. Political homophily in social relationships: Evidence from online dating behavior. The Journal of Politics, 79(1), pp.269-283.
Iyengar, S., Sood, G. and Lelkes, Y., 2012. Affect, not ideologya social identity perspective on polarization. Public opinion quarterly, 76(3), pp.405-431.
Iyengar, S. & Westwood, S. J. 2015. ‘Fear and Loathing Across Party Lines: New Evidence on Group Polarization’, American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.
Iyengar, S., Lelkes, Y., Levendusky, M., Malhotra, N. and Westwood, S.J., 2019. The origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Annual Review of Political Science, 22, pp.129-146.
Jackson, J. and Foster, J. 2021. The Politics of Public Budgeting in Illinois. The Simon Review. Paul Simon Public Policy Insitute. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1065&context=ppi_papers
Kalla, J.L. and Broockman, D.E., 2022. Voter Outreach Campaigns Can Reduce Affective Polarization among Implementing Political Activists: Evidence from Inside Three Campaigns. American Political Science Review, pp.1-7.
Kalmoe, N.P. and Mason, L., 2019, January. Lethal mass partisanship: Prevalence, correlates, and electoral contingencies. In National Capital Area Political Science Association American Politics Meeting.
Kalmoe, N.P. and Mason, L., 2022. Radical American Partisanship: Mapping Violent Hostility, Its Causes, and the Consequences for Democracy. University of Chicago Press.
Kiefer E. 2017. ’Til Trump do us part: the relationship deal breaker we never saw coming. Aug. 1, Refinery29. https://www.refinery29.com/2017/07/162856/talking-politics-with-partner- relationship-advice
Knudsen, E., 2021. Affective polarization in multiparty systems? Comparing affective polarization towards voters and parties in Norway and the United States. Scandinavian Political Studies, 44(1), pp.34-44.
Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press Lelkes, Y., Sood, G. and Iyengar, S., 2017. The hostile audience: The effect of access to
broadband internet on partisan affect. American Journal of Political Science, 61(1), pp.5-20.
Levendusky, M.S., 2013. Why do partisan media polarize viewers?. American Journal of Political Science, 57(3), pp.611-623.
Levendusky, M., & Malhotra, N. 2016. Does media coverage of partisan polarization affect political attitudes? Political Communication, 33(2), 283–301. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2015.1038455
Levendusky, M.S., 2018. Americans, not partisans: Can priming American national identity reduce affective polarization?. The Journal of Politics, 80(1), pp.59-70.
Mason, L., 2018. Uncivil agreement: How politics became our identity. University of Chicago Press.
Martherus, J.L., Martinez, A.G., Piff, P.K. and Theodoridis, A.G., 2021. Party animals? Extreme partisan polarization and dehumanization. Political Behavior, 43(2), pp.517-540.
McConnell, C., Margalit, Y., Malhotra, N. and Levendusky, M., 2018. The economic consequences of partisanship in a polarized era. American Journal of Political Science, 62(1), pp.5-18.
Mutz, D.C., 2002. Cross-cutting social networks: Testing democratic theory in practice. American Political Science Review, 96(1), pp.111-126.
Nicholson, S.P., Coe, C.M., Emory, J. and Song, A.V., 2016. The politics of beauty: The effects of partisan bias on physical attractiveness. Political Behavior, 38(4), pp.883-898.
Oliphant, B. and Smith, S. 2016. “How Americans are talking about Trump’s election in 6 charts,” Pew Research Center, 22 December 2016; www.pewresearch.org/fact- tank/2016/12/22/how-americans-are-talking-about-trumps-election-in-6-charts/.
Pacilli, M., Roccato, M., Pagliaro, S., and Russo, S. 2016. “From political opponents to enemies? The role of perceived moral distance in the animalistic dehumanization of the political outgroup.” Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 19(3), 360-373.
Pew Research Center. 2017. Most democrats, republicans have ‘just a few’ or no friends in opposing party. Available at http://www.people-press.org/2017/10/05/8-partisan- animositypersonal-politics-views-of-trump/8_02
Puglisi, R. and Snyder Jr, J.M., 2011. Newspaper coverage of political scandals. The Journal of Politics, 73(3), pp.931-950.
Rogowski, J.C. and Sutherland, J.L., 2016. How ideology fuels affective polarization. Political Behavior, 38(2), pp.485-508.
Rossiter, E. 2021. “The Consequences of Interparty Conversation on Outparty Affect and Stereotypes.” Working Paper. https:// erossiter.com/files/conversations.pdf.
Settle, J. E. 2018. Frenemies: How social media polarizes America. Cambridge University Press.
Sood, G. and Iyengar, S., 2016. Coming to dislike your opponents: the polarizing impact of political campaigns. Available at SSRN 2840225.
Tavernise, S. andSeelye, K. 2016. “Political divide splits relationships—and Thanksgiving, too,” The New York Times, 15 November 2016; www.nytimes.com/2016/11/16/us/political-divide- splitsrelationships-and-thanksgiving-too.html.
Perez-Truglia, R. and Cruces, G., 2017. Partisan interactions: Evidence from a field experiment in the United States. Journal of Political Economy, 125(4), pp.1208-1243.
Utych, S.M., 2020. A voter-centric explanation of the success of ideological candidates for the US house. Electoral Studies, 65, p.102137.
Vavreck. L. 2017. A Measure of Identity: Are You Wedded to Your Party? New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2017/01/31/upshot/are-you-married-to-your-party.html
Valentino, N., Brader, T., Groenendyk, E.W., Gregorowicz, K. and Hutchings, V.L.. 2011. “Election Night’s Alright for Fighting: The Role of Emotions in Political Participation.” The Journal of Politics 73 (01): 156–70.
Warner, B., & Villamil, A. 2017. A test of imagined contact as a means to improve cross- partisan feelings and reduce attribution of malevolence and acceptance of political violence. Communication Monographs, 84(4), 447–465. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2017.1336779
Webster, S.W. and Abramowitz, A.I., 2017. The ideological foundations of affective polarization in the US electorate. American Politics Research, 45(4), pp.621-647.
Westfall, J., Van Boven, L., Chambers, J.R. and Judd, C.M., 2015. Perceiving political polarization in the United States: Party identity strength and attitude extremity exacerbate the perceived partisan divide. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), pp.145-158.
Wojcieszak, M. and Warner, B.R., 2020. Can interparty contact reduce affective polarization? A systematic test of different forms of intergroup contact. Political Communication, 37(6), pp.789- 811.
YouGov. 2022. Do you think parents should be required to have their children vaccinated against infectious diseases?. https://today.yougov.com/topics/health/survey- results/daily/2022/02/17/1ad4c/1
[1] These are all just a sample of much more research that I exclude due to space.